Sweet child of mine


Babies in the womb react differently to flavors, according to a new Durham University study published recently

Babies in the womb are big fans of carrots but not so much leafy green vegetables—and show it in their faces, scientists said in a new study published recently.

Researchers at Durham University in northeast England said the findings were the first direct evidence that babies react differently to various smells and tastes before they are born.

A team of scientists studied 4D ultrasound scans of 100 pregnant women and discovered that babies exposed to carrot flavors showed “laughter-face” responses.

Those exposed to kale flavors in contrast showed more “cry-face” responses.

“A number of studies have suggested that babies can taste and smell in the womb, but they are based on post-birth outcomes while our study is the first to see these reactions prior to birth,” said lead postgraduate researcher Beyza Ustun. “As a result, we think that this repeated exposure to flavors before birth could help to establish food preferences post-birth, which could be important when thinking about messaging around healthy eating and the potential for avoiding ‘food-fussiness’ when weaning.”

Humans experience flavor through a combination of taste and smell. 

A team of scientists studied 4D ultrasound scans of 100 pregnant women and discovered that babies exposed to carrot flavors showed “laughter-face” responses. Those exposed to kale flavors in contrast showed more “cry-face” responses.

In fetuses, it is thought that this might happen through inhaling and swallowing the amniotic fluid in the womb. 

The study, published in the journal Psychological Science, included scientists from Durham’s Fetal and Neonatal Research Lab and Aston University in Birmingham, central England.

A team from the National Center for Scientific Research in Burgundy, France was also involved.

The teams believe the findings could deepen understanding of the development of human taste and smell receptors as well as perception and memory. 

 “It could be argued that repeated prenatal flavor exposures may lead to preferences for those flavors experienced postnatally,” said research co-author Professor Jackie Blissett of Aston University. “In other words, exposing the fetus to less ‘liked’ flavors, such as kale, might mean they get used to those flavors in utero. The next step is to examine whether fetuses show less ‘negative’ responses to these flavors over time, resulting in greater acceptance of those flavors when babies first taste them outside of the womb.” (AFP)